The Journal of Comparative Psychology (JCP): A Network Analysis of the Status of Comparative Psychology Daniel Lahham A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTERS OF ARTS GRADUATE PROGRAM IN PSYCHOLOGY YORK UNIVERSITY TORONTO, ONTARIO January, 2014 ? Daniel Lahham, 2014 Network Analysis of the JCP ii Abstract Comparative psychology?s relationship to various other sub-disciplines and scientific ?movements? has been discussed by many scholars throughout its history. The majority of these analyses took the form of frequency counts of the different subject species used within scientific periodicals (Schneirla, 1946; Beach, 1950; Dukes, 1960; Lockard, 1971) and presented similar conclusions: rats were the most commonly researched organism and the study of learning was the key to understanding behavior. The most popular of these critiques was Frank Ambrose Beach?s ?The Snark was a Boojum? (1950). Beach argued that comparative psychology, with the advent of behaviorism, slowly became a discipline focused only on rat learning in mazes. Donald Dewsbury (1984) responded to these discussions claiming that frequency counts alone could not depict the success and failures of the comparative discipline. Instead, he argued that comparative psychology maintained a historically continuous tradition of excellence off the efforts of a small group of prominent comparative psychologists. In this study, I attempted to ?bridge? the gap between these two competing views of the comparative discipline in order to view the legitimacy of both claims. Using network analysis, a tool common to digital history, I investigated metadata (organism studied, scientist, institution) from the Journal of Comparative Psychology during the period of 1911 to 1950. I found that both arguments were partially correct in their assertions. Comparative research was being conducted by a small group of prominent scientists throughout the entire four-decade period on many more species other than the rat; however, the broader comparative discipline was heavily impacted by the influx of research on learning in rats. In both cases, the authors inadvertently focused solely on their own claims, and failed to recognize the validity of the other. Network Analysis of the JCP iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract?????????????????????????????????????ii Table of Contents???????????????????????????????......iii List of Tables??????????????????????????????????..iv List of Figures?..????????????????????????????????...v Introduction???????.????????????????????????????.1 [Introduction]?????????????.??????????????????..1 [Historiography]?.?????????????????????????????.6 [Method]?????????????????????????????????...9 Chapter One: [First Generation of Psychobiology]..??????????????????..19 Chapter Two: [Second Generation of Psychobiology]?????????????????...60 Chapter Three: [Neo-Behaviorism].???.?????????????.?????????88 Conclusion???????????????????????????????????...137 Bibliography???????????????????????????????????.146 Appendices???????????????????????????????????..155 Appendix A: [Tables]?????.??????????????????????...155 Network Analysis of the JCP iv LIST OF TABLES Table 1: [Organisms (1911-1950)]??????????????????????..155 Table 2: [Scientists (1911-1950)]..??????????????????????..155 Table 3: [Organisms (1911-1930)]?...????????????????????...156 Table 4: [Organisms (1931-1950)]?..?????????????????????156 Table 5: [Institutions (1911-1950)]??????????????????????..157 Network Analysis of the JCP v LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: [Scientist ? Organism (1911-1950) ? First Gen.]????????????....39 Figure 2: [Scientist ? Organism (1911-1930) ? First Gen.]???????????.?...43 Figure 3: [Scientist ? Organism (1931-1950) ? First Gen.]????????????....45 Figure 4: [Institution ? Organism (1911-1950)]?????????????????....49 Figure 5: [Institution ? Organism (1911-1930)]?????????????????....51 Figure 6: [Institution ? Organism (1931-1950)]?????????????????....55 Figure 7: [Scientist ? Organism (1911-1950) ? New Gen.]????????????....73 Figure 8: [Scientist ? Organism (1911-1930) ? New Gen.]???????????.?...75 Figure 9: [Scientist ? Organism (1911-1950) ? New Gen.]????????????....77 Figure 10: [Scientist ? Organism (1920-1950)].????????????????.?..93 Figure 11: [Institution ? Organism (1920-1950)]?????????????????..94 Figure 12: [Scientist ? Organism (1920-1929)]?..????????????????..97 Figure 13: [Scientist ? Organism (1920-1929)]?????????????????....98 Figure 14: [Scientist ? Organism (1920-1929)]??????????????..???..99 Figure 15: [Scientist ? Organism (1920-1950) ? Hull & Tolman].??????????..102 Figure 16: [Scientist ? Organism (1920-1950) ? Top Rat Researchers]??.?????..109 Figure 17: [Scientist ? Organism (1920-1950) ? Rat Researchers Circled]??????..114 Figure 18: [Institution ? Organism (1920-1950) Rat Research Centers]???..????.122 Network Analysis of the JCP 1 Introduction On September 7, 1949, Frank A. Beach presided over Division 3 of the American Psychology Association ? the Division of Experimental Psychology ? and delivered an address as co-president.1Beach calculated the proportions of different organisms used as subjects in studies published in the Journal of Comparative Psychology and the two journals from which it had descended: Journal of Animal Behavior and Psychobiology. Beach argued that psychologists were basing their studies disproportionately on the rat in comparison to other organisms in comparative psychology and the entire psychological discipline at large2. He argued that for a discipline to be truly comparative, a wide variety of organisms must be used. The rat may indeed still be used but in addition to other mammals, vertebrates, and invertebrates; not the heavy concentration on a single organism that has historically occurred. Since comparative psychology sought to compare organisms, Beach questioned how one could claim to be a comparative psychologist and have one species predominate to such a degree. He further declared that this over-concentration had caused comparative psychology to ?suddenly and softly vanish away? (Beach, 1950, p. 115). Although offered comically and metaphorically, Beach?s address (and later article), entitled ?The Snark was a Boojum? affected the discipline beyond just the article itself: it set off a series of studies on the different organisms used as test species in the Journal of Comparative Psychology. Indeed, every decade since the address, at least one study has been conducted that ?took another look at the Snark? in order to determine the rat?s rise as a subject species (Dukes, 1 Beach was originally elected as President by Division 6 ? Division of Physiological and Comparative Psychology. After the merger between Division 6 and Division 3 in 1948 -1949, Beach joined W.J. Brogden as co- presidents of Division 3 (Dewsbury, 1996). 2 The reasons the rat became the standard laboratory are many and will not be discussed here. Network Analysis of the JCP 2 1960; Lockard, 1971; Porter, Johnson & Granger, 1981; Grossett & Poling, 1982; Adkins-Regan, 1990; Dewsbury, 1998; Shettleworth, 2009). However, although Beach?s concern has received the most attention, he was not the first to draw attention to this rat phenomenon; Theodore Christian Schneirla originally signaled this overuse of the rat in 1946. Indeed, Schneirla and Beach were close colleagues at the American Museum of Natural History during the 1940s (Dewsbury, 1984). This thesis is not an attempt to ?revisit? this Snark phenomenon, nor is it an attempt to explain why the rat (or any other species) came to be such a prolific laboratory animal. This has been done very well already by Cheryl Logan (1999; 2005). Furthermore, this thesis is not a chronological history on comparative psychology. Donald Dewsbury has written an exhaustive history of the discipline and remains one of the authoritative voices on comparative psychology. Instead, in this study, I chose to examine the historical claims made about the successes and failures of comparative psychology as a discipline, and its relationship with various other subdisciplines and scientific ?movements.? In particular, I attempted to determine how comparative psychology could have disappeared after the 1920s (Beach, 1950) whilst simultaneously prospering in the 1930s (Dewsbury, 1984).3 To explore these contradictory claims, and reconcile how both could exist simultaneously, I went beyond the assessment of model organisms alone, and included the organisms? experimental relationships with the researchers and institutions that studied them. Similar to the other ?Snark? articles, I extracted my data (organisms, scientists, and institutions) from articles in the Journal of Comparative Psychology. Although this journal did not encompass the entirety of comparative psychology?s breadth, it was one, significant marker of the field. Since I had access to every article published in the JCP, I utilized a ?digital? 3 Both Beach?s and Dewsbury?s claim were unpacked later in the thesis. Network Analysis of the JCP 3 approach for this project. The particular digital approach I employed was that of a network visualization that depicted the connections between organisms and the researchers and institutions that studied them. Through the structures of these networks, I argued that the claims made by Beach in 1950 and Dewsbury in 1984 about the status of comparative psychology as a discipline were both correct at root, but flawed insofar as they both inadvertently failed to recognize the validity of each other?s argument. There was an obvious ?data gap? between these two studies, which was likely a contributing factor to their opposite stances. Beach and Dewsbury used only partial runs of journals because they did not have the technology now available to handle large bodies of data. To bridge this gap, I included all the data in the specified time period, and I have expanded its breadth to include not only the organisms studied, but also the scientists and institutions that studied them. This allowed me to expand upon Beach?s data ? which included only odd numbered volumes, and focused solely on the frequencies of organism use and topics under study. Moreover, by expanding my data to include scientists and institutions in the study, I was able to use these pieces of data to dig deeper into the historical literature, bringing me closer to Dewsbury?s extensive qualitative analysis. Thus, my study merged the quantitative (Beach) with the qualitative (Dewsbury). As was mentioned previously, Beach (1950) contended that comparative psychology disappeared post 1920s, only to be replaced by behaviorism. Schneirla (1946; 1952) similarly argued Beach?s point, asking the question, ?Have We a Comparative Psychology?? in his critical evaluation of the discipline (Schneirla, 1952).Dewsbury (1984), on the other hand, was much more optimistic in his view of the discipline, claiming that the discipline prospered in a time when most others saw it on the decline. A reason for the discrepancy between these claims Network Analysis of the JCP 4 stemmed from how each author assessed the discipline: Beach and Schneirla interpreted the discipline by way of the most studied organisms (rats); and Dewsbury appraised comparative psychology through key events (conferences, publications, etc.) and prolific comparative psychologists (Robert Yerkes, Karl Lashley, Calvin Stone, Harry Harlow, and not least of all Beach and Schneirla). To demonstrate the accuracy of both claims on the status of comparative psychology, I assessed the influence of two competing, yet similar behavioral approaches: psychobiology and neo-behaviorism. Both approaches indicated the shift in American psychology from the study of internal mental states to the study of overt behaviors at the turn of the century. Moreover, both approaches directly connected to the arguments of Dewsbury, and that of Beach and Schneirla. Dewsbury argued that comparative psychology was maintained and prospered from of the efforts of a handful of prolific comparative psychologists. Since there is no consensus on what, precisely, identifies a comparative psychologist (Dewsbury, 1984), I likened Dewsbury?s ?comparative psychologists? to the tag of ?psychobiologist.?Two features bound the terms ?comparative psychologist? and ?psychobiologist?:4 the study of behavior through the lenses of psychology and/or biology; and the conducting of research outside of the behavioristic framework. Indeed, even Dewsbury himself used the two terms interchangeably on the same individuals (Robert Yerkes and Karl Lashley were both comparative psychologists and psychobiologists) (Dewsbury, 1984; 1991; 2002). Thus, I used the title ?comparative psychobiologist? to describe the individuals that Dewsbury claimed maintained the comparative discipline throughout the four decade period. I assessed these comparative psychobiologists at 4 ?Psychobiology? should not be confused with ?biopsychology.? Whereas ?psychobiology? is focused on the biological basis of behavior, ?biopsychology? is a branch of psychology that emphasis biological and physiological aspects (Dewsbury, 1991). Network Analysis of the JCP 5 the individual and institutional level to investigate whether or not they were indeed as prolific (in terms of quantity and quality) as Dewsbury had argued. Citing the disproportionate use of rats as subject animals (the model organism of choice in neo-behaviorism),5 and the rise in popularity of the maze learning studies akin to neo- behaviorism, Beach and Schneirla purported that behavioristic theory essentially replaced comparative psychology post 1920s. As such, I sought to determine the influence of neo- behaviorism in the JCP to explore the claim that it ?replaced? comparative research. To accomplish this, I examined the most historically prolific neo-behaviorists, and the researchers and institutions most closely connected to the rat (measured in terms of quantity). In order to determine the influences of both psychobiology and neo-behaviorism, the thesis comprised of three chapters. The first and second chapters covered the period from 1911 to 1950, and focused on the influence of the prominent comparative psychobiologists, indicated by Dewsbury, within the newly established Journal of Comparative Psychology. Moreover, the two chapters examined this era diachronically, breaking the time frame into two distinct periods, 1911 to 1930 and 1931 to 1950, in order to elucidate the different generations of comparative psychobiologists. The first chapter began by reviewing the literature on early comparative psychology in order to establish the initial purpose of the discipline of comparative psychology. Next, the first chapter examined the ?first generation? of comparative psychobiologists. The second chapter, then, investigated the ?new generation? of comparative psychobiologists. The third and final chapter assessed the time frame from 1920 to 1950 in a synchronic fashion, and examined the influence of neo-behaviorism in the JCP. Although this time frame was seemingly identical to chapter one and two, each chapter examined different sections within the overall 5 The term ?neo-behaviorism? refers to the reformulations in the 1920s, 30s, and 40s, of Watson?s Behaviorism of the 1910s. Network Analysis of the JCP 6 network. For example, whereas the networks in chapters one and two placed the emphasis on the number of organisms studied, the networks in chapter two placed the emphasis on number of studies conducted. In chapters one, two, and three, I utilized two different network constructions: the first network displayed the relationship between organisms and authors; and the second network portrayed the relationship between organisms and institutions. Throughout these chapters, I will show that Dewsbury was correct in claiming that comparative psychology (via psychobiology) was maintained by a handful of prominent individuals whilst also demonstrating that through the influence of neo-behaviorism (via the rat) in the JCP, Beach?s assertion that behaviorism replaced comparative psychology, although extreme in its proclamation, was also correct. Historiography Historians have often taken different approaches to writing the history of comparative psychology and the relationships between scientists and organisms. Moreover, each historian has their own distinct interpretation of the historical events they chronicle. For Beach (1950), the history of comparative psychology was a tragedy: comparative psychologists became too enthralled with the rat as a laboratory animal and stopped ?comparing? organisms. He appealed to the comparative psychologists of his time, imploring them not to do away with the rat, but to diversify the laboratory organisms they used. Beach went so far as to claim that an overdependence on the rat and learning studies had caused comparative psychology to disappear only to be replaced by behaviorism. In order to make these claims, he collected his data from odd-numbered volumes of the Journal of Animal Behavior, Psychobiology, and the Journal of Comparative Psychology. Beach was interested in which organisms were being studied and in the general topics of the research (e,g., learning, development, reproduction, etc.), of which he Network Analysis of the JCP 7 had seven categories. He presented the percentages of articles that used each organism in specified time frames, and the percentage of articles focused on each of his seven topic categories. For Beach, he was not so much concerned with the scientist, as he was with the organisms. Many other scholars and historians have echoed Beach?s sentiment and methodology. Porter, Johnson, and Granger (1981) continued Beach?s study, and examined the proportions of organisms being used in the Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology from 1961 to 1976. They reported a similar overdependence on the rat. Beach and Porter et al. both interpreted the history of comparative work through the lens of the most studied organism: the rat.6 Beach?s 1950 paper was an extension of Schneirla?s (1946) study in which he analyzed the articles in the Journal of Animal Behavior (JAB)from 1911-1917 and the articles in the JCP from 1938-1941 (Schneirla, 1946). Schneirla noted some striking findings in the data. The most dramatic finding was the increase in the percentage of articles with rats as subject species that went from 19% in the JAB to 66% in the JCP. Schneirla also showed that the percentage of studies on invertebrates dropped from 33% in the JAB to 5% in the JCP. Using this data, Schneirla claimed that comparative psychology was not very comparative at all and had changed with the advent of behaviorism. However, included in his analysis was a caveat that has oft-been overlooked by researchers conducting similar research (Schneirla, 1946; Dewsbury, 1984). Schneirla warned that the JAB was a highly interdisciplinary journal, and a large number of the studies were contributed by biologists. He further asserted that the majority of early research on the problems of instincts were conducted by zoologists; psychologists were more commonly studying learning and sensory systems (Schneirla, 1946). Dewsbury (1984) took a different approach and interpretation of the history of comparative psychology: he chronicled the sub-discipline?s history in its entirety, not focusing 6 Many others have continued this trend: Adkins-Regan, 1990; Dewsbury, 1998; Shettleworth, 2009. Network Analysis of the JCP 8 solely on its flagship journal. Moreover, he did not conduct the frequency analyses that Schneirla, Beach, and other ?Snark? scholars had done. For him, comparative psychology was not the tragedy that Beach and others had imagined. Instead, Dewsbury saw a new field in a fledging discipline striving to find and sustain success. He interpreted the history of comparative psychology by focusing on the organisms being studied, the important figures that helped build the field, and the major events that took place in the time period he covers. Unlike Beach, Dewsbury emphasized both the scientist and the organism. Moreover, Dewsbury broke down his chapters by each successive decade. His data was not explicitly displayed as it had been in Beach?s ?The Snark was a Boojum,? but was woven into the story he told. He did not write of proportions of certain organisms or percentages of certain types of studies. Instead, he drew attention towards certain landmark events in the discipline. Describing Dewsbury as an optimist would not be an overstatement. The periods in which many viewed comparative psychology as being on the decline, Dewsbury subscribed to an opposing position. For instance, Misiak and Sexton (1966) viewed the 1930s as a period of progressive decline in comparative psychology due to the rise of clinical psychology and the increasing specializations of other psychological fields. Dewsbury, on the other hand, viewed it ?as an outstanding decade in comparative psychology? (1984, p. 99). He argued that a new generation of comparative psychologists were produced that were some of the most prominent psychologists of the day. The 1940s had also been seen by earlier authors as a period of decline in comparative psychology (Schneirla, 1946; Scott, 1973). Once again, Dewsbury rejected this, arguing that ?the vigor that was generated in the 1930s was sustained in the 1940s? (1984, p. 117), mostly through the continued productivity of the ?new generation? of comparative psychologists. Although there were fewer newer faces in the 1940s, the established comparative psychologists were as active as ever. Dewsbury Network Analysis of the JCP 9 conducted a masterful synthesis of the key events, institutions, and figures in comparative psychology in the twentieth century, going beyond the JCP. Few scholars have gone to the lengths that Dewsbury did in his account of the discipline. Method Since this project dealt with a large amount of data (1911-1950), I chose to use digital methods that provide the network visualizations, which, when coupled with traditional methods, provide a powerful new interpretation or understand of the topic. A network is an example of one form of these visualizations. With initiatives in place to digitize large bodies of text, the number of sources available to historians has greatly increased (Rosenzweig, 2003). These large databases were the impetus for the development of new digital methods. One of the most appealing aspects of digital history is the sheer number of sources that can be included in a single analysis. Computational analysis allows one to analyze hundreds or even thousands of sources that would otherwise be impossible to examine closely even over the course of a single individual?s entire career. These computational methods often include computing word frequencies and carrying out various kinds of text mining. The present study benefited from these advantages in two ways: accessibility and size of database. The journal from which the data was drawn has been digitized and is available via the scholarly database PsycInfo. Thus, access to the primary literature was straightforward and efficient. The journals under consideration published over 2000 articles in the relevant time range, from which the metadata was retrieved. In order to analyze this ?big data,? computational methods were necessary. Many scholars have been moving towards answering historical questions by using digital tools that mobilize large quantities of data. Indeed, historians of science have shown an interest Network Analysis of the JCP 10 in developing new approaches that incorporate both close reading, in its traditional sense, and ?distant reading,? that is to say, the aggregation and analysis of large amounts of data (Moretti, 2005), which are now made possible with digital historical methods. The exponential growth of physics in post-Cold War America had led David Kaiser (2012) to embrace and advocate change in historical methods. Kaiser argues that this exponential growth has robbed the historian the privilege of solely relying on traditional tools of inquiry such as ?close-focus case studies, deep archival excavations, [and] microhistories? (Kaiser, 2012, p. 276). For him, ?these tools of inquiry seem to be no match for the brute fact of exponential growth ? the extraordinary expansion of people, places, and papers that has marked the scientific at least since World War II? (p. 276). Kaiser?s solution was a series of quantitative methods that determined the number of PhDs received and the size and number of journals, making clear patterns in isolated case studies, and making visible questions that can lead to close, archival research. Extracting large amount of data from open access databases ? which I have done in this thesis ? is not a new concept. A project conducted by Jane Maienschein and Manfred Laubichler (2010) utilized the open access database, PubMed, as a digital archive providing insight into the history of developmental biology. As is the case with most methodologies, there are limitations to accompany the many advantages. In the case of digital history methods, the limitations have been well documented by the same scholars that advocate its use. Caroline Winterer (2012) analyzed correspondence networks using Geographic Information System (GIS) mapping ? a system to analyze, manage and present geographical data ? in order to determine America?s place in the Republic of Letters ? the long distance community of intellectuals in the 17th century. However, Winterer proposes the caveat that ?digitizing humanistic data forces black-and-white answers onto the kinds of Network Analysis of the JCP 11 grey-area questions that historians usually delight in tackling? (Winterer, 2012, p. 598). Although this may sometimes be the case, I would argue that the networks continuous spatial layout allows for much finer investigations of the range of greys than one would be able to navigate with mere words. In any case, these limitations outlined by Winterer need not be debilitating as long as we keep in mind that the visualization alone does not provide instantaneous interpretative insights, but rather, is a guiding tool that informs the historian?s judgment. The visualizations used in this project were created by using important pieces of metadata coded from the articles and relied heavily on uncovering hidden and salient relationships through the study of the relevant literature (Pettit, Serykh, and Green, forthcoming/2015). Networks and Data The data for this project was collected by recording several important pieces of metadata from each substantive7 article in the Journal of Comparative Psychology: year, organism of choice, author(s), and institutional affiliation. The entire run of the journal was available from the scholarly database called PsycInfo. From each article, the year of publication, the type of organism used in the study, the name of the author(s) of the study, and author?s institutional affiliation were recorded, by hand, in an Excel spreadsheet. The spreadsheet was then imported into a program called Gephi that produces network images from datasets. A network graph is made up of two classes of components: nodes and edges. Nodes represent ?actors?(which can be nonhuman entities such as animals or institutions). Edges connect two nodes and represent particular kinds of relationships between those particular actors (e.g., node x used node y in research, x published y, etc.). For the present study, the nodes 7 In this case, a substantive article refers to an original empirical article. Reviews, notes, obituaries, and theoretical articles are excluded. Network Analysis of the JCP 12 represented the organism used, and either the first-author of the publication or the institution affiliated with the publication, depending on the particular kind of network. The edges connecting these nodes to represent two distinct relationships: either that between the author and organism, or that between the institution and the organism. Edges can be directed or undirected. Direction conceptualizes the overall network. If a relationship is considered directed, it purports that one node is acting upon another node in some way. For example, a relationship would be directed if one individual sends a letter to another. An undirected network assumes that directionality is unimportant in that particular relationship. Edges can also be weighted; weighted records something quantitative about the relationship between two nodes. For this particular project, the edges were weighted based on the number of publications a particular author had using a particular organism or the number of publications an institution had using a particular organism. In this thesis, I used a social network software called Gephi. Gephi reads its data from a CSV spreadsheet and can easily export back to a spreadsheet. Not only can Gephi provide aesthetically pleasing network visualizations, but it also provides informative measures for understanding the structure of the network. One of these measures is the idea of centrality within the network. Freeman (1979) classified three forms of centrality: degree, betweenness, and closeness. Degree centrality measures the number of edges that are connected to that particular node. This indicates the overall connectivity of that node within the network, and in my case, makes salient the most prolific authors, institutions, or organisms. A node?s degree is directly related to its visibility and importance within the network; the higher the degree, the more visible and important the node. The problem with degree, however, is that an edge that represents a single article by an author about a given organism is indistinguishable from one that represents Network Analysis of the JCP 13 five articles by one author about a given organism, unless the difference is represented by something we call ?weight? ? the thickness of the edge in the network. To resolve this problem, a second degree measure is used that measures the total weight of the links coming into a particular node. This measure, called ?weighted degree,? was used to determine the most prolific authors, institutions, and organisms in terms of output. Betweenness measures how often a particular node appears on shortest paths between every pair of nodes in the network (Brandes, 2001). It makes visible the nodes that serve as mediators between different parts of the network and is independent of a nodes degree centrality. This measure would be particularly fruitful in determining the most active employee in a workplace. Closeness centrality measures the average distance from a given node to all other nodes in the network (Brandes, 2001). This type of centrality would be most useful in analyzing correspondence networks. Because I am mostly concerned with which actors (nodes) were the most influential within the overall network, only degree centrality was used in my project (Pettit et al., forthcoming/2015). Although degree centrality is the simplest measure, it is often the most useful. Apart from degree centrality, I also used a node?s position in the network, often referred to as the spatial layout of the network, to assess the relationship between two nodes. The spatial layout of particular nodes in the network is highly dependent on both degree and weighted degree. How far a node is positioned from the center of the network depends on how many distinct connections it makes. The more connections a node makes, the more integral they are to the overall network structure. Moreover, weighted degree influences the force or strength a particular node has over nearby nodes. Two separate networks were created; one that covered the data from 1911 to 1950; and another that covered the period of 1920 to 1950. Moreover, the networks were constructed in two Network Analysis of the JCP 14 different ways: an author based network that represents an author-organism dyad, and an institution based network representing an institution-organism dyad. Source (JCP) The data was taken from the Journal of Comparative Psychology (JCP) from the period of 1911 to 1950. The data consisted, specifically, of organisms, authors, and institutions. This is the current title of the journal (since 1983), but it has operated under three other titles ? Journal of Animal Behavior (JAB), Psychobiology (PSB), and Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology (JCPP) (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987; [Cover], 1947). The journal originally began as the JAB and ran from 1911 to 1916. After the journal could not financially sustain itself, it was dropped and effectively replaced by PSB in 1917. PSB would not last long, printing only two volumes in 1917/18 and 1920, before changing names to the JCP. The JCP was established in 1921 as the combination of the interests of the JAB and the PSB. The JCP operated under the editorship of Knight Dunlap until 1943, when one of his colleagues, Roy M. Dorcus, took over editorial duties. Dorcus held editorship until 1947, when Calvin Perry Stone became editor and the journal?s name changed to the JCPP. In 1983, the journal split into Behavioral Neuroscience and the reinstated JCP. Journal of Animal Behavior The establishment of this journal owed much to the diligent work of Robert Yerkes. Indeed, the JAB is often considered to be Yerkes? journal (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987). At the turn of the century, the study of animal behavior was gaining momentum. Yerkes, who established the animal psychology laboratory at Harvard and arranged a book series focused on animal behavior with the Macmillan Company, grew impatient with the lack of a journal specifically dedicated to publish animal work. He sought the advice of the zoologist, Herbert Spencer Jennings, about Network Analysis of the JCP 15 establishing such a journal in 1903, a year after he graduated with his doctorate. Jennings suggested that the Carnegie Institute of Washington might fund the endeavor, but it appeared that Carnegie had other intentions with their money (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987). Yerkes was forced to move in a different direction, becoming an editor for the Journal of Comparative Neurology (JCN) in 1904. The JCN was founded by C.L. Herrick in 1891 as ?a quarterly periodical devoted to the comparative study of the nervous system (Herrick, 1891). Yerkes joined the likes of Herrick, his brother C.J. Herrick, and O.S. Strong, and was put in charge of all work published by the journal in comparative psychology and animal behavior; topics that the journal newly adopted. This change in the journal?s scope was indicated by the change of its title to the Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology (JCNP) in 1904.8 Unfortunately, most journals were not self-supporting enterprises at this time and the new JCNP was in financial peril. Yerkes had made many attempts to solve the issue, but faced too many impediments (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987). The journal needed more subscriptions but faced heavy competition from the Biological Bulletin and the newly established Journal of Experimental Zoology (JEZ) in 1904. By 1906, Jennings and many other collaborators of the JCNP had felt that animal behavior work was better suited for the JEZ. Yet, the JEZ suffered the same financial difficulties as the JCNP. Both would be purchased by the Wistar Institute of Anatomy and Biology in Philadelphia in 1907 along with the Journal of Morphology, the American Journal of Anatomy, and the Anatomical Record. Jennings and Yerkes saw this centralization as a chance to better concentrate materials on animal behavior; unfortunately, this hope was not realized, and both men began looking for another way of concentrating animal research. Although Jennings was full of doubt that an animal behavior journal could self-support, Yerkes pursued the matter in earnest in the following years (Burkhardt, 1987). 8 For a more detailed account of the reasons and justifications for this change, see Burkhardt, Jr., 1987. Network Analysis of the JCP 16 By January, 1910, plans were in place for a new journal, and Yerkes and Watson had begun the process of selecting an editorial board. Although many names were suggested, the editorial board ultimately consisted of prominent zoologists S.J. Holmes (Wisconsin), Jennings (Johns Hopkins), and W.M. Wheeler (Harvard), and significant psychologists M. Bentley (Illinois), H.A. Carr (Chicago), E.L. Thorndike (Columbia), Watson (Johns Hopkins), M.F. Washburn (Vassar), and Yerkes (Harvard), who served as managing editor ([Cover], 1911). The JCNP announced the new Journal of Animal Behavior late in 1910 (Herrick, 1910). Although the editors of the JCNP expressed regrets that they were losing Yerkes and his two associates, Watson and Jennings, they wished the new journal success, and recommended that any papers regarding comparative psychology or animal behavior be sent to the JAB. In 1911, eight years after Yerkes had first expressed to Jennings the need for a journal for work on animal behavior, the JAB published its first issue. The JAB would soon face financial issues of its own, which led to its discontinuation in 1917. Yerkes had managed to keep the journal afloat through 1915 based on contributions from the editors themselves, but with escalating printing costs in 1916, the journal ran a deficit. Yerkes discussed the idea of having the Wistar Institute take over financial responsibility for the journal, but the initiative was fruitless. Even raising the subscription price from $3.00 to $5.00 was not enough to save the journal. Financial concerns were not the only reason the journal did not survive. The JAB rested on the efforts of Yerkes, and in 1916 and 1917, Yerkes took on many other initiatives that occupied up much of his time. He served as president of the APA in 1916/1917; he organized the Army intelligence testing program during the war; and he chaired the Psychology Committee of the National Research Council (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987). Psychobiology Network Analysis of the JCP 17 In May 1917, Knight Dunlap approached Yerkes with the suggestion that the Journal of Animal Behavior be merged with his soon to be established periodical, Psychobiology (PSB). Yerkes immediately rejected the offer, claiming that it would not be advantageous to combine the interests of the two journals (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987). Nonetheless, PSB published its first issue in July 1917. It ran two volumes, one in 1917/1918, and the other in 1920. In his opening editorial, Dunlap said that the journal?s primary aim was ?the publication of research bearing on the interconnection of mental and physiological functions? which included work on ?physiological psychology?pharmacology, physiology, anatomy, neurology and psychiatry in so far as the results of these investigation have explicit bearing on problems of mental life, or mental factors are included in the essential conditions of the investigation? (Dunlap, 1917, p. 1). In addition to Dunlap (Johns Hopkins), the journal?s editors were J.J. Abel (Johns Hopkins), W.B. Cannon (Harvard), R. Dodge (Wesleyan), S.I. Franz (Government Hospital), H.S. Jennings (Johns Hopkins), and G.H. Parker (Harvard). Journal of Comparative Psychology In 1921, the JCP was established, replacing both JAB and PSB (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987). Yerkes and Dunlap served as co-editors for this journal; although Dunlap served as managing editor and took primary control of the journal. Up till this point, Yerkes was still heavily involved with his chairmanship of the Research Information Service of the National Research Council. The remainder of the editorial board consisted of the amalgamation of the editors of the JAB and PSB. The JCP would continue publishing until its name change in 1947. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology In 1947, the JCP was renamed the Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology ([Cover], 1947). Calvin P. Stone served as the new managing editor for this journal, joined by an Network Analysis of the JCP 18 impressive list of consulting editors: F.A. Beach (Yale), C.W. Darrow (Chicago), R.M. Dorcus (California), H.F. Harlow (Wisconsin), W.T. Heron (Minnesota), H. Kl?ver (Chicago), K.S. Lashley (Yerkes Laboratories), C.T. Morgan (Johns Hopkins), E.C. Tolman (California), and G.R. Wendt (Rochester) ([Cover], 1947). The journal would run under this name until it split into Behavioral Neuroscience and the Journal of Comparative Psychology (for a second time) in 1983. It is not well documented why the journal?s name was changed to include physiology in its title. According to historian Don Dewsbury, ?the field was changing and the physiological types, growing rapidly, felt that the name no longer was appropriate for their work? (Dewsbury, 2012, email correspondence). Network Analysis of the JCP 19 Chapter One: First Generation of Psychobiology in the Journal of Comparative Psychology Determining a date that marked the beginning of comparative psychological work is a difficult task and a debate on its own. Dewsbury (1984) chose to begin his history with a prehistory dating back nearly 34 000 years. Eventually, he designated the year 1894, not as the beginning of comparative psychology, but as his preferred starting point. Although Dewsbury considers 1894 to be somewhat of an arbitrary choice, his decision was guided by important intellectual events of the year, some of which were: the death of von Helmholtz and Romanes; Margaret Floy Washburn took her PhD at Cornell under Titchener; John Dewey moved to the University of Chicago and M?nsterburg was at Harvard; James McKeen Cattell and James Mark Baldwin established the Psychological Review as an alternative outlet to the house organ The American Journal of Psychology; and the year 1894 saw the publication of C. Lloyd Morgan?s An Introduction to Comparative Psychology. Following Dewsbury, I chose 1894 as the most appropriate starting point for this project. 1894-1899 As Dewsbury aptly suggested, the year 1894 was important for the emergence of comparative psychology. American Psychology was preparing for the explosive growth that the turn of the century would bring: twenty-nine psychology laboratories had been established across the United States by 1894 (Garvey, 1929). In the same year, G. Stanley Hall, a staunch advocate for the grounding of scientific psychology in the model of Darwinian evolutionary theory, was serving in his sixth year as President of Clark University. Baldwin, another proponent of evolutionary theory in psychology, was at Princeton where the year prior he established his second psychological laboratory (the first was at Toronto in 1890). Baldwin later reopened the Johns Hopkins laboratory in 1903. One of Baldwin?s most important contributions to the study Network Analysis of the JCP 20 of evolution, and by extension comparative psychology, was his proposal of ?organic selection? in 1896. This process was later dubbed the ?Baldwin effect? (Simpson, 1953). Essentially, the ?Baldwin effect? explained the process by which a nonhereditary (acquired) trait might come to be hereditary without resorting to Lamarckian mechanisms. Also in 1894, Baldwin teamed up with James McKeen Cattell to found the Psychological Review, a journal to rival the The American Journal of Psychology. Baldwin and Cattell originally proposed one of two options to Hall: buying The American Journal of Psychology from him, or at least establishing a board of editors with powers to accept and reject submissions. Hall refused both options even though he admittedly had grown tired of the duties of an editor (Sokal, 1997). The Psychological Review held importance for comparative psychology, since much early comparative work was published in this journal. A journal dedicated solely to comparative work, however, was not established until the Journal of Animal Behavior (JAB) in 1911. The year 1894 also saw significant events in Europe that helped shape comparative psychology. It was in this year that Wilhelm Wundt?s Lectures on Human and Animal Psychology was translated by J.E.E. Creighton and E.B. Titchener and first published in English. Wundt described two different approaches to animal psychology: a comparative approach where animals (including humans) were studied for the purpose of understanding the development of the organic mind, and an alternative approach in which animals were studied solely for their utility in understanding the human mind. Not only did Wundt?s Lectures have a profound influence on the legitimacy of animal psychology and the method of objective behavioral observation, but his recognition of a purely comparative approach with no reference to its human relevance was an important step in establishing the field of comparative psychology (Dewsbury, 1984). This prompted Warden?s remark that ?even so conservative a writer as Network Analysis of the JCP 21 Wundt?recognized the right of comparative psychology to develop a content independent of human reference in the same sense as zoology is independent of human morphology and physiology? (Warden, 1928, p. 514). In England, C. Lloyd Morgan?s publication of An Introduction to Comparative Psychology has been referred to by Dewsbury as ?the most memorable event? of 1894 (Dewsbury, 1984, p. 45). The book outlined trial-and-error learning, ?constraints? on learning, and accepted the idea that perception should be viewed from the organism?s perspective. Morgan?s book rejected the view of Larmackian9 inheritance of acquired traits (Dewsbury, 1979; 1984). A staunch supporter of the study of instinctual behaviors, Morgan wanted to separate inherited behavior from learned behavior. For him, every species has its own instinctive behaviors that do not have to be learned; the discussion of ?consciousness? in animals can only be had if the behavior cannot be explained by any other way. Indeed, Morgan?s canon states: In no case may we interpret an action as the outcome of the exercise of a higher psychical faculty, if it can be interpreted as the outcome of the exercise of one which stands lower in the psychological scale (Morgan, 1894, p. 53). Morgan would figure prominently in the coming post-World War I debates on instinct. A few years later, at Clark University, Linus Kline and Willard S. Small conducted the first psychological tests of rats in mazes, an event that set the standard for animal psychology and comparative psychology in the twentieth century (Dewsbury, 1984). Kline first came to Clark University in 1896 to study ?zoological psychology.? Interested in what he termed home sickness, Kline studied homing in bees, pigeons, and chicks. Dewsbury describes Kline?s use of rats and mazes as a somewhat serendipitous event. Kline was working with chicks and reading 9 Lamarckian inheritance or Lamarckism is the process of an organism passing on a trait to its offspring that was acquired, not inherited. It was named after the French biologist and evolutionary theorist Jean-Baptiste Lamarck (1744-1829) (Sapp, 2003; Gould, 2002). Network Analysis of the JCP 22 Morgan?s Introduction when the idea of trapping rats in small boxes to study their food searching behavior occurred to him. The inspiration for the use of rats came from a Clark colleague, biologist Colin C. Stewart, who studied the effects of alcohol, diet, and barometric pressure on rat behavior. By the fall of 1898, Kline had devised a study in which rats learned how to get food from ?mouse boxes.? On December 3, 1898, Kline undertook his first study of learning in rats (Dewsbury, 1984). A little over a month later, on January 9, 1899, Kline conducted a study on social interactions, in which two rats had to interact to secure food from a box. The use of a maze, however, was the idea of Edmund C. Sanford, professor of psychology and founding director of the Clark psychology laboratory (Sokal, 1990). Kline described to Sanford ?runways [he] observed several years ago made by large feral rats to their nests under the porch of an old cabin? (cited in Miles, 1930, p. 331). Sanford promptly suggested to Kline the use of the Hampton Court maze. Kline subsequently modified the design out of an encyclopedia, and the first maze was constructed by Small. Small noted in his study on rat maze learning (Small, 1901) that the natural propensity of the rat to navigate winding passages was a critical reason for their decision to study rats using mazes. In terms of the first use of white rats, Stewart began his studies using wild, ?gray? rats before moving onto tame white rats. On his switch, he wrote, ?If anyone wants to know why I changed from wild gray rats to white rats in 1895, let him work with gray rats for a year? (cited in Miles, 1930, p. 334). Stewart got his white rats from C.F. Hodge, a biologist specializing in neurology and physiology at Clark (Baldwin, 1901). Around the same time, another neurologist and student of Hall?s, Henry H. Donaldson, began a colony of white rats at the University of Chicago. Donaldson eventually established the Wistar Institute of Anatomy and Physiology in Philadelphia that became famous for their Wistar strain white rats. The Wistar Institute would come to supply many of the most active psychology Network Analysis of the JCP 23 laboratories in the United States. Further information on Donaldson?s and the white rat?s influence on psychology is provided by Logan (1999; 2001; 2005). The individuals who would later impact comparative psychology were just beginning their studies in 1894: Robert Mearns Yerkes was an undergraduate at Ursinus; Edward Lee Thorndike was an undergraduate at Wesleyan; and John Broadus Watson was a freshman at Furman University. Edward L. Thorndike moved from Wesleyan to Harvard in 1896 to begin his graduate studies. Working with young chicks in the cellar of William James? home,10 Thorndike?s Harvard research on the instinctive nature of pecking behavior in chicks was published in 1899 (Thorndike, 1899a). Thorndike did not stay long at Harvard, leaving in 1897 to complete his PhD at Columbia. Dewsbury cited personal and financial reasons as the cause for the move. In 1898, at Columbia, Thorndike completed his thesis entitled, Animal Intelligence: An Experimental Study of the Associative Processes in Animals, which immediately became a classic study in the history of animal learning. The text primarily dealt with learning in dogs, cats, and chickens. Thorndike famously used his puzzle boxes that required an animal to interact with a lever or other device in order to escape the box and secure food. Thorndike found that the animals solved problems in a trial and error fashion, and did not display reason. His thesis was so influential that the results were summarized and published in Science (1898) and in the article ?Do Animals Reason?? in Popular Science Monthly (1899b). Thorndike also conducted one of the first ever studies of learning in fish, demonstrating escape learning in killifish (Fundulus) (Dewsbury, 1984). 10 Thorndike?s landlord would not permit the chicks in his room and James was unable to secure laboratory space. Thus, Thorndike resorted to conducting his studies in the cellar of William James? home. Network Analysis of the JCP 24 Evidently, this six-year period was extremely important in the development of a comparative psychological discipline. The future giants of the field were nearing the start of their influential careers: Yerkes entered graduate school at Harvard and in 1899 transferred from zoology to psychology; and Watson was one year removed from his historic move to the University of Chicago. In 1899, Sanford at Clark, and G.H. Mead at Chicago, began offering courses in comparative psychology. Moreover, the first two comparative psychology laboratories were placed by Warden and Warner (1927) at Clark, under Sanford, and Harvard, under Yerkes. 1900-1909 The twentieth century began much the same way as the nineteenth century ended, with a large degree of optimism towards comparative psychology. As C. Judson Herrick proclaimed in 1907, ?comparative psychology [had] arrived? (Herrick, 1907, p. 76). Further to the optimism that surrounded comparative psychology, this was a time when psychology moved towards experimental design over naturalistic observations, and when anecdotal evidence was replaced by data derived from experiments (Yerkes, 1943; Dewsbury, 1984). It was also a time when learning theory started to gain serious traction in comparative psychology. Still, the comparative psychologists at the time understood the importance of natural observation and of studying different aspects of behavior (Dewsbury, 1984). Some of the first learning studies at the turn of the century belonged to Small (1900; 1901) and Thorndike (1901). Using Kline?s puzzle boxes, Small placed white rat?s on the outside and food in the inside of the box. This was opposite to what Thorndike had done previously, placing the animal in the box and the food outside. Small, like Kline, was extremely vigilant about keeping animals in the most naturalistic environment as possible. This was due in large part to the orientation of Small compared to Thorndike; Small believed animals do things ?only Network Analysis of the JCP 25 in line of its inherent abilities? (Small, 1900, p. 133), and thus, drew inspiration from Morgan, whereas Thorndike cited Lubbock and preferred a laboratory method approach (Dewsbury, 1984). In Small?s study, the rat had to dig into the box to get the food, and the digging behavior was considered in line with the natural characteristic of the rat. In Thorndike?s study, having the animal trip a lever or latch to attain food might not have been as naturalistic a setting, but gave the researcher the desired experimental control. As learning theory became more dominant in the coming decades, Thorndike?s approach became the precedent, and only a few remained loyal to the more naturalistic approach that Small advocated. In 1899, Thorndike began researching his fourth species after receiving three cebus monkeys and housing them in his residence on West 123rd Street in New York (Joncich, 1968; Bitterman, 1969). Similar to the conclusions derived from his thesis, Thorndike found that the monkeys did not show any signs of reasoning or imitation behavior. Furthermore, although Thorndike did admit that the monkeys could learn to solve problems other mammals could not, he attributed this to a difference of degree, not one of kind. This conclusion that learning did not differ substantially across a wide diversity of species had serious implications for the future of comparative learning studies, and animal psychology in general (see Beach, 1950; Logan, 2001). During the first decade of the twentieth century, three institutions led the way on comparative studies on learning: Harvard, Clark, and Chicago. The Harvard animal lab was headed by Yerkes, a Harvard pupil who had just graduated with his doctorate in 1902. Yerkes, who figures prominently in the next chapter, studied learning in a wide range of species, including turtles (1901); green crabs (1902); frogs (1903); crawfish (Yerkes and Huggins, 1903); and dancing mice (1907). His master?s student, Lawrence W. Cole (1907) added raccoons to the list of species being studied by those affiliated with Harvard (Pettit, 2010). Although much of Network Analysis of the JCP 26 Yerkes? early work was on learning, he was interested in a whole range of behavioral patterns. He would become renowned for his work in the field of primatology and his championing of the chimpanzee as a model organism. The Clark laboratory was headed by Sanford and, although much of the early work was done by Small and Kline, other Clark workers were conducting comparative experiments as well. A.J. Kinnaman (1902), for instance, conducted a laboratory experiment on rhesus macaques. The monkeys were expected to interact with and manipulate a number of devices. Unlike Thorndike, who claimed there was no evidence of reasoning or imitation in his studies with monkeys, Kinnaman found that the monkeys did exhibit reason and imitation. According to Dewsbury (1984), Kinnaman was arguably the first to study monkeys in a laboratory setting. However, credit for the first use of monkeys belongs to Thorndike. Another Clark student, James P. Porter, conducted comparative learning studies on spiders (1906a), and on English sparrows, vesper sparrows, and cowbirds (1904, 1906b). Porter also studied ?instinctual? behaviors such as mating, web building, and feeding in spiders. One of Sanford?s doctoral students, Herbert Burnham Davis, studied intelligence in raccoons, along with a variety of instinctive patterns. Alongside Cole and Walter S. Hunter, Davis was one of three major figures in the history of raccoon research (Pettit, 2010). Cole and Davis both began studying learning in captive raccoons in 1905, and although conducted separate experiments, both studies had much in common. Both utilized the newly developed puzzle boxes on a novel organism, with the intention of assessing the raccoons? ?comparative place on the scale of animal intelligence? (Pettit, 2010, p. 399). Meanwhile, John B. Watson received his doctorate in 1903 at Chicago, and remained there as an instructor. The majority of Watson?s work was done primarily on white rats: his well- received dissertation (Watson, 1903) was a study on the behavioral development of white rats. Network Analysis of the JCP 27 This study initiated Watson?s meteoric rise in psychology. However, there were those who were critical of Watson?s method of sacrificing rats for brain studies. Watson?s work began with the caring of Donaldson?s colony. He was not, however, limited to rats only. In 1906, he solicited the university to purchase for him four monkeys. The monkeys were used for a study of imitation (1908). Like Kinnaman, Watson believed that imitation existed, and he expressed the importance of further studies on the phenomenon. In the years 1905 and 1906, the careers of Watson and Yerkes intersected via a correspondence, before diverging sharply in later years (Watson became a radical behaviorist while Yerkes called himself a psychobiologist). The importance of this interaction is highlighted by the status of both men as future giants in the field of comparative psychology. Furthermore, Watson?s early allegiance with Yerkes was contrasted by the shaky relationship between the Harvard and the Clark labs. Indeed, Yerkes believed that although comparative psychology was alive and well at Clark, it was ?indiscreet or bad form for a Harvard psychologist to try to cultivate friendly professional relationships with Hall and his Clark associates? (Yerkes, 1943, p. 75). This was due in part to the long-standing rivalry between William James and Hall, as well as differences in the publishing habits of the two institutions: Harvard workers tended to publish brief papers focused on learning in animals; Clark workers published longer articles that included extensive introductions, life histories, and points on methodology (Dewsbury, 1984). And although Watson did not engage in many comparative studies, his student, C.S. Yoakum (1909), conducted learning studies on squirrels and compared them to other species. By the end of the 1890s, comparative psychology laboratories had been established at Harvard and Clark and comparative courses offered at Chicago and Clark. By 1910, comparative courses were offered at Cornell, Drake, Harvard, Ohio State, Iowa, Johns Hopkins, Michigan, Network Analysis of the JCP 28 Mount Holyoke, Oklahoma, and Texas. Moreover, comparative psychology laboratories were established at Chicago, Cornell, Johns Hopkins, Michigan, and Texas (Warden &Warner, 1927). Evidently, comparative psychology was growing rapidly in America during this decade. Watson proclaimed that ?comparative psychology has completely justified its existence? (1906, p. 155). He was wary, however, that many topics had been breached and not enough refined. Coupled with this lack of refinement was the inadequacy of the available facilities. Thus, with the support of Baldwin, Watson called for an experimental station devoted to the study of animal behavior (Dewsbury, 1984): The need to the psychologist of an experimental station for the study of the evolution of the mind is as great as is the need to the biologist of an experimental station for the study of the evolution of the body and its functions (Watson, 1906, p. 156). 1911-1950 At the turn of the century, the discipline of psychology was changing. Psychologists were concerned with their scientific status, leading to a divide in regards to methodology, subject matter, and goals of the discipline (Burnham, 1968; O'Donnell, 1979, 1985; Samelson, 1977). Not only was psychology in the process of emancipating itself from philosophy, but it was also assessing its distance from biology, and determining the degree to which its utility was able to justify its existence. Under the pressure of these circumstances, Robert Yerkes and John Watson, among others, defined and developed an experimental science based on the study of animal behavior, culminating in the establishment of the Journal of Animal Behavior in 1911; one of the first institutionalizations of the study of animal behavior (Burkhardt, Jr., 1987). However, although Yerkes and Watson were both advocates for the experimental study of overt animal behavior, they both took similar, but different approaches. In 1913, Watson published his Network Analysis of the JCP 29 ?Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It? in the Psychological Bulletin. This important publication essentially became the behaviorist manifesto. Watson called for psychologists to leave behind the attempt to study subjective, conscious mental states by means of introspection, replacing it with the objective experimental study of overt behaviors (Cohen, 1979; Dewsbury, 1984; Benjamin, 2007).Watson envisioned the development of a complete program that would emphasize the study of behavior in nonhuman organisms and eventually extend its principles to human behavior.11 Behaviorism or late-Watsonian behaviorism soon thereafter came to resemble an extreme reductionist12 approach, eliminating any influence of inherited factors. Even though Yerkes similarly sought to distance himself from psychology as the study of mental experience, he did not subscribe to the behavioristic framework purported by Watson and others. Instead, Yerkes moved towards psychobiology ?as an integrative discipline with physiology? (Dewsbury, 1991, p. 200), and established a program of comparative psychobiology at Harvard (Yerkes, 1930). This research orientation allowed Yerkes to study overt animal behavior without being shackled to the increasingly popular behaviorism. Yerkes considered himself a psychobiologist rather than a psychologist. Indeed, his autobiography was titled ?Robert Mearns Yerkes: Psychobiologist? (Yerkes, 1930). Yerkes was not the first individual to use the term psychobiology. In fact, Dewsbury (1991) has traced the earliest use of the word to Willard S. Small (1901) in his paper that began research on maze learning. Knight Dunlap also considered himself a psychobiologist in the same way that Yerkes did. However, the term psychobiology has had many diverse meanings throughout history. Yerkes and Dunlap distinguished between three different clusters of uses for 11 The fourth chapter will discuss behaviorism in more detail. 12Reductionism is a philosophical position that holds that a phenomenon is nothing more than the sum of their parts, and the object of study could be understood by its individual constituents (Jones, 2000; 2013) Network Analysis of the JCP 30 psychobiology: the first one was the way it was used by E.S. Russell; the second cluster was Adolf Meyer?s use of the term; and the third their own meaning of the term. Edward Stewart Russell was a prominent British biologist whose Form and Function (1916) sought to change classic morphology into a functional morphology. For Russell, the organism ? that he perceived as purposive ? had to be studied holistically, including both the morphological with behavior. He termed this approach ?hormic biology? (Russell, 1923; 1924). His use of psychobiology came in his opposition to vitalism. Russell argued that it was purposiveness that distinguished between living and non-living things and that purpose was studied through a psychobiological (functional) method in biology. The Swiss-born psychiatrist, Adolf Meyer was a long time professor of psychiatry and director of the Phipps Clinic at Johns Hopkins University. He used psychobiology to define his holistic approach to psychiatry as the integration of social, psychological, and biological factors to better treat patients (Meyer, 1915). Psychobiology was a way for Meyer to deal with ?the functions of the whole person and not merely as detachable parts? (Meyer, 1915, p. 861). Dewsbury (1991) also noted that most definitions of psychobiology define it in the way Meyer used it. This is not surprising considering the influence Meyer had on Dunlap, Watson, and many others in this era (Dewsbury, 1991; Billings, 1939). In 1914, Knight Dunlap published a textbook entitled, An Outline of Psychobiology. The book most closely resembled what would today be called physiological psychology (Dewsbury, 1991). Dunlap believed that ?[he] raised this term from its grave, and employed it as covering the consideration of biological materials and theories from the point of view of the bearings of these on the problems of psychobiology? (cited in Dewsbury, 1991, p. 199). Dunlap also founded and served as executive editor of Psychobiology, a periodical that published two volumes in 1917 Network Analysis of the JCP 31 and 1920. Psychobiology, for Dunlap, was the interconnection between mental and physiological functions, and could also be called ?physiological psychology.? Moreover, Dunlap?s psychobiology also encompassed investigations in pharmacology, physiology, neurology, anatomy, and psychiatry as long as these investigations had bearing for problems in mental life (Dunlap, 1917). Although Yerkes accepted Dunlap?s use of psychobiology, his research interests were much broader and his work was less physiological and more behavioral. Yerkes also applied the term to distance himself from mainstream psychology, in particular, behaviorism. It appeared that Yerkes and Dunlap at least believed they were using the term similarly (Yerkes, 1934). However, there appears to be little continuity in the way they used the term compared to Meyer?s use. Dunlap, in particular, believed that Meyer used the term in a philosophical manner, which he claimed bore no resemblance to his usage of the term. Meyer echoed these differences in how he used psychobiology compared to Dunlap and Yerkes. He believed Dunlap?s use was too narrow and academic, and indicated the difference between him and Yerkes was the latter?s acceptance of mental causality (Dewsbury, 1991). For the purposes of this study, I used Dunlap and Yerkes?s integrated definition of psychobiology. That is to say, ?psychobiology? as an integration between psychology and physiology, and as a way to distinguish behavioral research from behaviorism. Before beginning the analysis of Dewsbury?s claims on the status of comparative psychology in the twentieth century, it is important to unpack some of his arguments. He was not convinced by the type of frequency counts employed by Schneirla, Beach, and other writers who revisited the ?Snark.? For Dewsbury, the percentage of comparative research studies on the lab rat did not matter nearly as much as the quality of comparative research and its impact on the wider discipline. He believed that focusing on quantity and percentages alone actually served to Network Analysis of the JCP 32 distort the image on the history of comparative psychology. His main thesis in the book however, was that although comparative psychologists were a small part of the wider psychological discipline, their achievements were noteworthy. Indeed, he argued that these same comparative psychologists developed and maintained the comparative discipline, and provided reason to be proud of the history of the discipline rather than apologetic. He asserted that a community of comparative psychobiologists maintained ?a consistent thread? of excellence throughout the history of the comparative discipline. Although Dewsbury acknowledged the achievements of many prominent comparative psychologists, he focused mostly on a handful of prominent psychobiologists that were highly impactful in maintaining comparative psychology. According to Dewsbury, during the 1910s, three individuals were key contributors to the development and establishment of comparative psychology, Yerkes, Watson, and the newly arrived Karl Spencer Lashley. However, although early a proponent of the study of animal behavior and comparative psychology, and highly influential in the development of the comparative field, Watson moved away from comparative psychology in the mid 1910s towards human based research. Dewsbury recognized the early 1920s as a transition period for comparative psychology. Lashley had just taken up a position at the University of Minnesota where one of his students and future significant comparative psychobiologist, Calvin Perry Stone, graduated. But mostly, the early 1920s was a time ripe with controversy over instincts and behaviorism, with many (neo) variations on behaviorism being offered. However, Dewsbury claimed that the return of Yerkes to academia in 1924, coupled with the influence of Lashley and the graduation of Stone, H.C. Bingham, and Carl J. Warden provided the boost necessary for comparative psychology to prosper in the late 1920s and early 1930s. Network Analysis of the JCP 33 It was Dewsbury?s characterization of the 1930s that was most distinct from other historians. Historians like Misiak and Sexton (1966) described the 1930s as a time of decline in comparative psychology. Moreover, Beach claimed that the 1930s saw the greatest rise in the percentage of research articles on the rat. However, Dewsbury argued that the graduation of a ?new generation? of prominent comparative psychobiologists made this decade one of the most notable in the history of comparative psychology. Including Yerkes, Lashley, and Stone, Dewsbury acknowledged the importance of the arrival of Norman Raymond Frederick Maier, Harry Friedrich Harlow, Winthrop Niles Kellogg, and Frank Ambrose Beach in the 1930s. These comparative psychobiologists were also largely responsible for Dewsbury?s characterization of the 1940s as a decade of sustained vigor. Dewsbury?s argument is predicated on the achievements of these prominent comparative psychobiologists. It is for this reason, then, that I have chosen to track these individuals throughout the period of 1911 to 1950 in the JCP. Although Dewsbury has accurately depicted the qualitative prominence of these individuals? achievements, I argue that these comparative psychobiologists were also quantitatively prominent. I measured their quantitative prominence by assessing how many organisms they published on in the JCP, and the total number of publications they contributed to this journal, both at the individual and institutional level. Thus, I conclude that Dewsbury was correct in his assessment of comparative psychology as a small, yet vigorous discipline. I decided to focus on two generations of comparative psychobiologists over two chapters. The first generation, covered in chapter one, included Yerkes, Lashley, and Knight Dunlap. The second generation, covered in chapter two, was made up of Stone, Maier, Harlow, Kellogg, and Beach. Dewsbury did not consider Dunlap a comparative psychologist because he studied mostly humans. However, I argue that his close connection to the JCP as executive Network Analysis of the JCP 34 editor, his self-identification as a psychobiologist (Dewsbury, 1991), and his research on four different organisms not only qualified him as a comparative psychobiologist, but made Dunlap one of the most influential individuals in comparative psychology?s history. My argument will be constructed as follows: I begin with a biographical sketch of the three ?first? generation comparative psychobiologists and then assess their individual and institutional influence on comparative psychology via the information derived from the networks. The higher their influence in the network, the more confident I am in arguing for Dewsbury?s characterization of comparative psychology as a small, important discipline maintained by a handful of prominent psychobiologists. Network Visualizations In this section, I introduce visual networks that depict the metadata collected from the Journal of Comparative Psychology (JCP) from its inception in 1911 to the year 1950. The metadata collected from the JCP was obtained strictly from substantive articles (reviews, notes, and theoretical papers were excluded) and included: year, organism studied, first author,13 and institutional affiliation. The data was then organized in a spreadsheet and imported in the software Gephi. Two different forms of networks were created, one depicting the relationship between organism and author, and the other depicting the relationship between organism and the institution. For this project, I constructed my argument based on three aspects of the network. The first aspect was assessing the authors and institutions connected to the most diverse array of organisms. This is measured using degree. A high degree is indicative of many distinct connections, and a low degree is indicative of very few distinct connections. In this project, a high degree represents the use of a comparative research approach or the use of a wide diversity 13 Including any co-authors distorts the quantitative measures in the network. If a co-author was prominent enough, they would eventually appear as a first author. Network Analysis of the JCP 35 of organisms. The second aspect was the total number of connections (not necessarily distinct) between an author/institution and the organisms they studied. This is measured using weighted degree, which takes into account all instances of a relationship between an author/institution and the organisms. The higher the weighted degree, the more articles were published by an author or institution. Thus, a high weighted degree indicates the most prolific authors/institutions in terms of output. The third and final aspect was the determination of a particular author?s position in the network. This is often referred to as the spatial layout of the network. I expect that the most comparative authors, connected to the most organisms, would be positioned nearest to the center of the network. In contrast, the authors connected to only one organism would be positioned more peripherally in the network. I argue that authors positioned near the center of the network were more integral to the overall structure of the network, and most likely to have been mediators or conduits between different organism clusters. Reading the networks from the vantage point of these three aspects allowed me to infer certain things about the comparative psychobiologists under study. For instance, a high degree indicated a researcher had studied multiple organisms in a particular time frame, and thus, had been comparative in their research program. In the context of my argument, studying multiple organisms was an important marker of a comparative psychobiologist. Weighted degree indicated which researchers were the most prolific or productive contributors (quantity) to this journal in a specified time frame. These productive researchers, at least quantitatively, were some of the most active in the field, and thus, were influential in the maintenance of the comparative discipline. Degree and weighted degree are not only useful measures for assessing the specified researchers at the individual level, but they also contribute to the structural positioning of a particular researcher in the overall network. Degree dictates how far a researcher Network Analysis of the JCP 36 will be positioned from the center of the network and how many different communities they connect, and weighted degree influences the force or strength a particular researcher has over nearby nodes. For example, if a researcher node has a higher weighted degree than an organism node, the researcher node will have a stronger gravitational pull. In this study, I expected the specified comparative psychobiologists to have been highly comparative (high degree) as well as having been prolific contributors to the journal (high weighted degree). With these high centrality measures, I also expected them to be positioned as mediators, connected to multiple communities near the center of the network, with stronger than average gravitational pull. The First Generation: Robert Mearns Yerkes, Knight Dunlap, and Karl Spencer Lashley Robert Yerkes was arguably the most important individual in the establishment and maintenance of comparative psychology. Not only did he establish the Journal of Animal Behavior, but his efforts helped develop an experimental program of comparative psychobiology in an era dominated by behavioristic theories. Yerkes was first educated at Ursinus College, receiving his A.B. degree in 1897. In the same year, he moved to Harvard where he took his A.B. degree in 1898, and his A.M. degree in 1899. Interest in zoology and psychology led Yerkes to comparative psychology and, at the suggestion of Josiah Royce, Yerkes transferred over to the psychology department to work in animal psychology. He received his PhD in 1902 under Hugo M?nsterberg (Yerkes, 1930) and subsequently founded the Harvard laboratory of comparative psychology (Dewsbury, 1984). Yerkes remained at Harvard in various roles -- assistant, instructor, and professor -- for the next fifteen years. As the Great War broke out, Yerkes accepted a position at the University of Minnesota as chair of the psychology department, which was newly separated from the philosophy department, but he never physically went there. Yerkes was integral in mobilizing psychologists during World War I, and remained as an administrator Network Analysis of the JCP 37 after the war had ended. In 1924, he moved to Yale as Professor of Psychobiology, where he remained until his retirement in 1942. Like Yerkes, Knight Dunlap was also a self-proclaimed psychobiologist who was extremely influential to comparative psychology and the JCP (Dewsbury, 1991). His education began at the University of California where he received his BPhil in 1899. From there, he moved to Harvard where he took his PhD in 1903. He encountered many of the same mentors and influences as Yerkes did (M?nsterberg, Royce, Dewey, and James) since both were at Harvard during the same period. In 1904, he took a faculty position at Berkeley before moving to Johns Hopkins University in 1906. He remained there for three decades, leaving in 1936 to establish a psychology laboratory and graduate program at the University of California, Los Angeles (Dorcus, 1950; Moore, 1949; Dewsbury, 1984). Dunlap was also vehemently against the current definitions of instincts;14 the only one of the three psychobiologists with this disposition. Coupled with his doctrine that good research resulted from assuming the opposite stance, Dunlap was an unpopular iconoclast (Dorcus, 1950). The third comparative psychobiologist, Karl Spencer Lashley, was different from the other two in that he never self-identified as a psychobiologist. Rather, the label was applied to him by the historian, Donald Dewsbury (2002). Dewsbury?s justification was, first, that his initial training was a PhD in zoology that he obtained under Herbert Spencer Jennings at Johns Hopkins University in 1914. Thus, much of Lashley?s work was biologically oriented. And second, through a series of learning experiments in the 1920s, Lashley dismantled the stimulus response theories of the day. Clearly, he was interested in behavior but not sympathetic to the behaviorist approach. He was, however, influenced by Watson during his graduate training, 14 The concept of ?instincts? have been an important and controversial debate throughout the entire history of the psychological discipline (see Dewsbury, 1984 for a detailed account). Network Analysis of the JCP 38 conducting field studies with Watson on terns on the Dry Tortugas in 1913. After attaining his doctorate, he worked with Shepherd Ivory Franz before joining the Minnesota faculty in 1917. He remained there until he went to the Institute for Juvenile Research (affiliated with the University of Illinois in Chicago) in 1926. His next stop was the University of Chicago in 1929. He left for Harvard in 1935, remaining there for the remainder of his career until 1955. While at Harvard, from 1942 to 1955, he held the position of director of the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate Biology. Psychobiologists in the Network The first network I looked at included the entire period of 1911 to 1950 (Figure 1). The network was weighted by using the number of organisms studied (degree). In other words, emphasis was placed on the number of organisms one studied rather than the number of studies published. Before determining how these three psychobiologists were depicted in the network, it is important to describe what one is seeing. This network consists of 760 nodes (authors and organisms) and 894 edges. Often, an author might have been studying multiple organisms, indicating why there are more edges than nodes in this network. Of the 760 nodes, 57 were organisms and 703 were authors. From a ?bird?s eye? perspective, the network appeared as a collection of clusters of various sizes. The largest cluster was made up of 348 distinct authors studying the rat for a total of 747 published articles on the organism (degree=348, weighted degree=747). The next largest cluster consisted of 127 distinct authors studying the human and publishing a total of 185 articles (d=127, wd=185). This trend of the rat and human being the two most studied organisms was seen throughout all versions of the networks. Table 1 presents the fifteen largest organism clusters in terms of degree and weighted degree, and Table 2 shows the top fifteen most comparative and productive researchers. 39 Network Analysis of the JCP 40 The three comparative psychobiologists, Yerkes, Lashley, and Dunlap, boasted not only a comparative approach, but were some of the most highly productive researchers in the JCP from 1911 to 1950. In terms of studying multiple organisms, Yerkes had the most comparative approach of any other author in the JCP, studying eight different organisms: rat, human, chimpanzee, monkey, dove, worm, pig, and bird. Lashley studied five different organisms: rat, bird, cat, monkey, and chicken. And Dunlap studied four different organisms: rat, human, bird, and rabbit. All three psychobiologists studied both birds and rats, Yerkes and Lashley examined monkeys, and Dunlap and Yerkes ran experiments on humans. (Yerkes? experiments on humans came during his time in the war effort). Despite the commonalities in species choice, each of the three had their own preferred organism: Yerkes favored the chimpanzee; Lashley preferred the rat for his neurological and perception research; and Dunlap felt that studying the psychological conditions of humans were most fruitful. Some might argue that Dunlap?s preference of the human made him a non- comparative psychobiologist (see Dewsbury, 1984), but I argue that his studies on the rat, bird, and rabbit displayed his comparative breadth. Yerkes and Lashley both began by studying a number of different organisms before ?finding? and sticking with their respective favored model. Dunlap took a different approach, beginning his studies on the human, moving to the rat, rabbit, and bird, before eventually returning to the human. Yerkes believed that anthropoids were the closest organism to humans, and thus, chose the chimpanzee as the ideal subject to investigate sexual and social behavior for the ?welfare of mankind? (cited in Biehn, 2008, p. 21). Lashley?s research revolved around neurological bases of learning, anatomical structures of the brain, sensation and perception, and the effects of drugs on learning (Hebb, 1959). The small, abundant, and inexpensive rat was the most amenable and logical organism for this type of Network Analysis of the JCP 41 research. Dunlap?s interest in the field of vestibular research ? which he picked up from a brief stint in the army ? led to his and his students? choice of human subjects (Dorcus, 1950). In terms of the quantity of research published in the JCP, these three psychobiologists also were among the top. From 1911-1950 Karl Lashley published 14 articles, Robert Yerkes contributed 12 papers, and Knight Dunlap authored 11. The majority of each researcher?s repertoire, however, was focused on their preferred standard organism, establishing a close connection between the two. Along with degree, this close connection contributed to their positioning as close to the node representing their desired organism. For instance, Yerkes? strong connection to the chimpanzee positioned the two very nodes close to one another in the network. Moreover, since Yerkes employed a highly comparative approach, their representative nodes were located closer to the center of the network than the periphery. Similarly, Lashley?s close connection with the rat kept him close to that species than any of the other four he studied. His connection to five different organisms also kept him close to the center of the network. Finally, Dunlap?s affinity to the human brought him close to that species cluster, and his degree of four kept him nearer the center. As I mentioned earlier, I argue that the closer a node (in this case a researcher) is to the center of the network, the more influential to the overall structure of the network and of the discipline itself. When the network is weighted on degree, in order to be near the center, a node must have multiple connections throughout the network. Thus, scientists near the center of the network are connected to multiple organism clusters, acting as conduits that presumably mediate the flow of information between communities of researchers and organisms. If we look at the degree and weighted degrees of these psychobiologists, and by consequence, their position in the network, we can argue that not only were these researchers some of the most comparative and productive in the discipline, but they were also mediating information between Network Analysis of the JCP 42 different ?communities? of researchers and organisms. In this respect, Yerkes, Lashley, and Dunlap were integral in maintaining the comparative discipline. However, such a large time period (1911-1950) does not allow us to make any claims on smaller time intervals. The Early Years (1911-1930) The period from 1911 to 1950 provided a general view of the discipline over several generations. However, it was difficult to discern any information about the early years when viewing the data over such a long time frame. For this reason, I split the time period of 1911- 1950 in half, focusing on the early years from 1911 to 1930, and the later years from 1931-1950. The network covering the period of 1911 to 1930 (Figure 2) consisted of 313 nodes and 335 edges. This new, smaller network, comprised of two dominant clusters, the human (d=71, wd=100) and the rat (d=70, wd=119), a number of smaller clusters (cat, monkey, chicken, dog), and a few dyadic and triadic clusters (see Table 3 for full degrees and weighted degrees of fifteen largest organism clusters). Similar to Figure 1, this network was weighted by degree, and thus, a researcher studying diverse organisms would once again be positioned nearer to the center of the network. In Figure 2, Yerkes, Lashley, and Dunlap were circled to make them easier to spot. The majority of Yerkes?s work came within the years of 1911 to 1930 (only three articles were published by Yerkes in the JCP after 1930). Even in this smaller network, Yerkes (d=8, wd=9) had the highest degree of any author, and thus, he was positioned near the center of the network. His high occurrence with the chimpanzee kept him closest to that organism, rather than the other organisms he studied, like the human or the rat. The opposite occurred as well: whereas the chimpanzee, monkey, pig, bird, dove, and worm influenced where Yerkes was positioned, he also influenced where these organisms were positioned in the network. 43 Network Analysis of the JCP 44 Indeed, Yerkes had a higher degree and weighted degree than the chimpanzee, worm, pig, bird, and dove. In simpler terms, Yerkes, an author, was connected to more species than the number of distinct researchers that were studying either one of the previously mentioned organisms. A node with a higher degree has a higher gravitational force in the network, drawing closer nodes with smaller degrees. Similar to Yerkes, the majority of Lashley?s publication output came in the period of 1911 to 1930. With a degree of five and weighted degree of 12, Lashley published twelve articles in this period, with only two articles on the rat published in the JCP after 1930. The comparative Lashley was also located near the center of the network, however, his nine articles on the rat kept him closer to that organism. Lashley was also connected to the relatively large cat, monkey, and chicken clusters. Dunlap, in opposition to the other two, was equally active before and after 1930, in the JCP. From 1911 to 1930, Dunlap studied three organisms for a total of six articles. Of the three authors, he was the only one who did not study the rat in this network. Instead, he conducted four studies on the human, and one each on the bird and rabbit. His studies on three organisms placed him in the center of the network, but closer to the human cluster. In this period, all three psychobiologists were positioned in the network to have presumably been mediating the flow of information between multiple organism communities. The Later Years (1931-1950) Figure 3 depicts the next time period, from 1931 to 1950. This network consisted of 526 nodes and 591 edges. The rat cluster exponentially eclipsed the human cluster in this time frame. The rat (d=293, wd=628) was studied nearly eight times more than the next closest organism, the human (d=61, wd=85). (Table 4 depicted the degrees and weighted degrees of the top fifteen organism clusters). The rise of the rat could be indicative of the spread of neo-behavioristic theory in the 1930s ? even though the rise of the rat as a subject species was a discipline wide 45 Network Analysis of the JCP 46 phenomenon (see Logan, 2002, 2005) ? or could simply be an artifact of more data in this network than the other. The increase in the number of articles published in this period caused each cluster to grow as well, but none reached the rat?s growth rate: in the period after 1930, four times as many researchers studied the rat, and five times as many articles were published on the rat (all figures taken from the JCP) compared to the next closest organism. Since the majority of Yerkes?s and Lashley?s work was done prior to 1931, it was no surprise to find that they each had a degree of one and a combined weighted degree of 5 (Yerkes published three articles and Lashley published two). Indeed, Yerkes retired as director of the Yale Laboratories of Primate Biology (YLPB) in 1942, and began writing his magnum opus, Chimpanzees: A Laboratory Colony (Biehn, 2008).15 He also resumed his position in the war effort during World War II (Dewsbury, 1984). Lashley succeeded Yerkes as the new director of the Yale Laboratories of Primate Biology in 1942 and conducted much important primate work himself (Carmichael, 1959). However, none of this work on primates was published in the JCP. In fact, Lashley stopped publishing in the journal after he moved to Harvard in 1935. Even though Lashley and Yerkes did not publish in the JCP in these later years, their respective ?standard? organisms, the rat and chimpanzee, continued to be studied more often. Knight Dunlap was fairly active, studying the human and the rat during this time period, and producing a total of five articles. Although he was not one of the more prolific contributors in this network, Dunlap?s connection to the rat and human positioned him as one of the few authors who mediated the two largest clusters in the network. Viewing the network in three different time periods, I was able to determine the extent and timing of these psychobiologists? influence in comparative psychology. Robert Yerkes, the founder of the Journal of Animal Behavior and the initial driving force of a comparative 15 For a full account of Yerkes?s retirement, see Biehn, 2008. Network Analysis of the JCP 47 psychobiology, studied the most diverse array of organisms of any other researcher. In terms of production, Yerkes conducted twelve studies, which was modest amongst the leaders. By breaking the time period in half, it became apparent that the vast majority of Yerkes? work came prior to 1930, explaining his modestly high output. Karl Lashley exhibited a similar trend to Yerkes insofar as most of his research output came in the period before 1930. Lashley contributed a total of fourteen articles on five organisms. Dunlap was less comparative and prolific overall than Yerkes and Lashley, publishing eleven articles on four organisms. However, unlike Yerkes and Lashley, Dunlap was equally as active before and after 1930. With this information in hand, it appeared Dewsbury was astute in characterizing Yerkes, Lashley, and to a lesser extent, Dunlap, as the driving force for the development and establishment of a comparative discipline in the 1910s and its maintenance in the transitional period of the 1920s. Indeed, after the 1930s, Dewsbury placed the success of comparative psychology in the hands of the newly graduated generation of psychobiologists. Up till now, we have only looked at one type of network depicting the relationship between researcher and organism. Thus, we have been able to determine the influence of these comparative psychobiologists on an individual basis. In order to determine their influence at the institutional level in developing and maintaining the discipline, a different type of network needed to be used depicting the association between organisms and institutions. The next section tracked trends in organism choice at the institutions these psychobiologists were affiliated with. In particular, I sought to determine how much of an institution?s experimental approach to the study of animals (comparative or a single model organism) was attributed to the psychobiologist they employed and whether or not the prominence of that institution was associated with the prominence of their resident psychobiologist. I argue that if the institutions that employed Network Analysis of the JCP 48 Yerkes, Lashley, and Dunlap were prominently contributing to the JCP in a similar manner to their in-house comparative psychobiologist (prominence measured by quantity of publications and the diversity of organisms studied), then the psychobiologists along with their institutions were influential in sustaining the comparative discipline. Institutional Affiliation of the Psychobiologists There was a large amount of overlap between the institutions at which these three psychobiologists researched and taught, and thus, their careers were interconnected. For instance, at the same time Yerkes took his doctorate at Harvard, Dunlap began his graduate training at the same institution. The connections did not stop there, however. Dunlap held a position as professor of psychology at Johns Hopkins when Lashley arrived as a student. When Lashley graduated from Hopkins, in 1917, he was hired by Yerkes as an instructor at Minnesota. Although Yerkes did not physically go to Minnesota, he still made hiring decisions. Indeed, 1917 was a busy year for Yerkes, who assumed the presidency of the APA, and chair of the Committee on the Psychological Examination of Recruits where he oversaw the administration of the Army Alpha and Beta tests (Yerkes, 1930; Benjamin, 2007). Yerkes moved to Yale in 1924 where he would spend the rest of his career; the latter portion of which was spent as director of the YLPB. Lashley went from Minnesota to the Institute for Juvenile Research in 1926, before taking a position at the University of Chicago in 1929. Later, Lashley went to Harvard in 1935 ? making all three psychobiologists affiliated with Harvard at some point in their careers ? and stayed until his retirement in 1955 (Hebb, 1959). He also succeeded Yerkes as the director of the YLPB. Dunlap was at Johns Hopkins until 1936 when he moved to UCLA to establish a graduate program in psychology (Zusne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